Online Read Free Novel
  • Home
  • Romance & Love
  • Fantasy
  • Science Fiction
  • Mystery & Detective
  • Thrillers & Crime
  • Actions & Adventure
  • History & Fiction
  • Horror
  • Western
  • Humor

    Why People Die By Suicide

    Page 8
    Prev Next


      He had also had the opportunity to habituate to pain and provo-

      cation in general. People work up to the extreme act of death by sui-

      cide through various means. The clearest involves previous suicidal

      behavior. But other means are possible too—any activity that allows

      people to get used to pain and provocation can serve to reduce fear

      of injury in general and self-injury in particular. Crane had plenty of

      opportunities to habituate to pain and provocation. He could not

      control his drinking, and frequently had drunken episodes that in-

      volved physical fights or the destruction of property. He spent time

      in jail in three different countries. Crane also had perhaps hundreds

      of anonymous sexual experiences, picking up sailors at New York’s

      docks. Anonymous sex might qualify as a provocative experience if

      some of the experiences turned violent, which seems likely given his

      history of alcohol use and drunken violent behavior.

      Crane’s life and death are clear examples of some of the themes of

      this book. For instance, many people who die by suicide appear to

      engage in short-term practice—I noted for example that my dad cut

      his wrists before the lethal wound to his heart. By contrast, through

      years of frequent provocative and painful experiences, people like

      Crane do not need short-term practice; they just go. And in fact, wit-

      nesses described Crane as “vaulting” over the rail of the ship. Crane’s

      lifetime of pain and provocation left him with no hesitation about

      death by suicide.

      Another fact about Crane is important, and it is that he character-

      ized a relationship that intensified near the end of his life as “some-

      thing of a reason for living.” The relationship was with the wife of a

      friend—their marriage had neared its end, and in the wake of it, and

      in the wake of Crane’s many troubles, a deep relationship emerged.

      Crane was gay, and it is not clear whether or not this relationship was

      sexual, though it was intense enough that Crane entertained ideas of

      marriage. In the days before his death, there were serious disruptions

      The Ability to Enact Lethal Self-Injury Is Acquired ● 57

      in this relationship. Crane had developed the capacity for self-injury

      already, and used it once his one remaining close relationship ap-

      peared to be falling apart.

      I have pointed to two of the three key components of completed

      suicide regarding Crane’s death—acquired ability to enact lethal self-

      injury (the focus of this chapter) and thwarted belongingness (a

      focus of the next chapter). The third component—a deep sense of

      incompetence or ineffectiveness (also a focus of the next chapter)—

      can be detected as well. In the days before his death, he said to many

      people that he felt “utterly disgraced,” in part by his drunken behav-

      ior on the ship, but also by his long history of such behavior.

      The death by suicide of the actor Spalding Gray in early 2004 is

      still another illustration of some of these principles. Gray was last

      seen on January 10 in New York City and was reported missing on

      January 11. There were reports that a Staten Island Ferry worker be-

      lieved he saw Gray coming off the ferry on the night of January 9.

      This left Gray’s wife and brother with the fear that the January 9 ferry

      ride was a “dry run” to prepare for the next day’s suicide. Their fears

      were confirmed when Gray’s body was found in the East River two

      months later, on March 7.

      Gray had attempted suicide multiple times since a 2001 car acci-

      dent in which he was badly injured. In September 2003, Gray left a

      phone message for his wife saying goodbye and indicating his plan to

      jump from the Staten Island Ferry. His wife called police, who radi-

      oed the ferry; ferry workers found a dejected Gray and escorted him

      off the boat.

      His lethal attempt was different. Before Gray was found, his wife

      stated that if her husband’s disappearance involved suicide, it had a

      different character than previous attempts. His past attempts had al-

      ways involved a note telling her what he would do, where he would

      be, and so forth. For his lethal attempt, there was no note; he just

      suddenly disappeared.

      There are other aspects of Gray’s death that are instructive. As was

      58 ● WHY PEOPLE DIE BY SUICIDE

      mentioned, he was badly injured in a car accident approximately

      three years before his death; he sustained a severely broken hip as

      well as head injuries. The experience of having been injured—having

      to endure the pain; facing the fear of bodily damage—may have in-

      ured Gray to the pain and provocation of self-injury. Just as Hart

      Crane habituated to pain and provocation through an array of pro-

      vocative experiences, including previous suicidal behavior, Gray’s in-

      juries, combined with his subsequent suicide attempts, may have left

      him prepared to fully face down death on his final attempt.

      Gray’s wife reported that his depression, which had been severe

      and treatment-resistant following his car accident, seemed to have

      been lifting in the days and weeks prior to his death. A well-known

      piece of clinical lore cautions that there is a window of heightened

      suicide risk as people emerge from depression, perhaps because they

      have the energy and cognitive clarity to act on long-standing suicidal

      ideas. There are anecdotal reports that appear to support this possi-

      bility. For example, Alvarez noted that the poet Sylvia Plath experi-

      enced increased energy and artistic productivity during the period

      before her suicide.20 This possibility was also noted—memorably—

      by the psychologist Paul Meehl in his famous paper “Why I Do Not

      Attend Case Conferences,” in which he describes upbraiding a stu-

      dent. Meehl, incredulous, asks the student if he has never heard that

      a psychotically depressed patient is more likely to kill himself when

      his depression is lifting. The student says no.

      “Well you have heard of it now,” says Meehl. “You better read a

      couple of old books, and maybe next time you will be able to save

      somebody’s life.”21

      The diminution of fear through repeated self-injury is, accord-

      ing to my account, necessary for serious suicidal behavior to occur.

      Shneidman described the case of Ariel,22 a young woman with previ-

      ous suicidal behavior (e.g., an overdose) who went on to attempt to

      burn herself to death. Her plan was to fill a gallon jug with gasoline,

      The Ability to Enact Lethal Self-Injury Is Acquired ● 59

      douse the inside of her parked car and herself with the gasoline, and

      then strike a match. She wrote, “I remember kind of shaking when I

      was getting the jug because I think I was a bit afraid.” Fear thus sur-

      faced even in Ariel, a woman who was clearly resolved to die by sui-

      cide. Ariel did strike the match, and was terribly burned. She sur-

      vived, but a few years later, died from natural causes.

      To be competent at and courageous about anything, one must

      have experience with it—the more experience, the more compe-


      tence and the more courage. The implications of continued experi-

      ence with provocative or painful stimuli, such as self-harm, are far-

      reaching.

      First, with repeated exposure, one habituates—the “taboo” and

      prohibited quality of suicidal behavior diminishes, and so may the

      fear and pain associated with self-harm. Second, and relatedly, oppo-

      nent processes may be involved. Briefly, opponent process theory23

      predicts that, with repetition, the effects of a provocative stimulus

      diminish, and the opposite effect, or opponent process, becomes

      amplified and strengthened. For example, with repeated use, the eu-

      phoric effects of heroin (the “a” process) weaken, and the aversive

      effects of withdrawal (the opponent process) increase; similarly, with

      repetition, the fear-inducing effects of skydiving (the “a” process)

      diminish, and the exhilarating effects of the opponent process are

      amplified. Skydivers become more competent and more courageous

      with skydiving practice and experience increasing reinforcement

      (e.g., exhilaration).

      So may suicidal people become more competent and courageous

      with repeated practice at suicidal behavior, and may even experience

      increasing reinforcement. Indeed, as will be expanded on later, many

      patients report that self-harm has calming and pain-relieving ef-

      fects—they self-injure because it distracts them from even deeper

      emotional pain, or because it makes them feel alive, or because it

      brings their inner world back into harmony with the world at large.

      60 ● WHY PEOPLE DIE BY SUICIDE

      What is the evidence that, through practice and the attendant ac-

      crual of competence and courage regarding suicide, people “work

      up” to the ability to enact lethal self-injury and may even find sui-

      cidal behavior increasingly rewarding? The several anecdotal exam-

      ples described in this chapter are consistent with this view, but anec-

      dotal evidence, by itself, is not particularly persuasive. If the view

      espoused here is true, what facts should be empirically demonstra-

      ble? In the following sections, several lines of research are described

      that, considered together, suggest that this viewpoint has merit.

      Multiple Suicide Attempts

      Alvarez wrote, “It is estimated that a person who has once been to

      the brink is perhaps three times more likely to go there again than

      someone who has not. Suicide is like diving off a high board: the first

      time is the worst.”24 Indeed, if past experience with suicidality facili-

      tates future suicidality, such that it becomes more serious and more

      lethal, people with multiple past suicide attempts should be demon-

      strably different from others, even including those with one past sui-

      cide attempt. My colleagues and I compared 134 current suicide

      ideators, 128 people who had recently made their first suicide at-

      tempt, and 68 people who had recently attempted suicide for at least

      the second time (i.e., multiple attempters). We compared the three

      groups on an array of symptom and personality indices. It should

      be noted that patients in all three groups were in crisis—they had

      either recently attempted suicide or ideated about it to the point that

      a mental health professional became concerned—and so the three

      groups did not differ in terms of why they were included in the study.

      All were suicidal in one way or another. A unique feature of this

      study was the comparison of multiple attempters to one-time at-

      tempters. The three groups did not differ with regard to age, so any

      differences among them were not likely age-related.

      And there were differences among them. As compared to those

      The Ability to Enact Lethal Self-Injury Is Acquired ● 61

      with suicidal ideas and those with one attempt, multiple attempters

      experienced more intense suicidal symptoms, such as desire to die,

      plans to attempt, resolve to die, intensity and duration of suicidal

      ideation, and so forth. This was the case on both self-report and cli-

      nician-rated scales of suicidality, which is important because the two

      data sources do not always agree (when they do, confidence in the

      results is higher). There were also differences on some personality

      variables, such as hostility. Even though all participants were in a

      suicidal crisis, multiple attempters stood out from others in terms

      of the severity of their suicidality as well as some features of their

      personality.25

      They had more past practice at suicide, and thus had moved fur-

      ther along the trajectory toward serious suicidal behavior than the

      others. Their position on this trajectory is indicated by the severity of

      their current suicidal symptoms. Other research groups, too, have af-

      firmed this pattern of findings.26

      What does the association between past suicide attempts and cur-

      rent suicidality mean? For example, it is possible that repeated sui-

      cidal behavior occurs simply because of an ongoing, chronic mood

      disorder. To rule out explanations like these, studies need to first doc-

      ument that an association between past and current suicidality exists,

      but also document that it persists when variables like chronic mental

      disorders are accounted for.

      In fact, several studies have shown that past suicidal behavior con-

      fers risk for later suicidality, including death by suicide, taking into account other key variables like mood disorders, for example. One

      study compared those who died by suicide to living controls. Sui-

      cides and controls were matched for presence and severity of mental

      disorders (also for gender and age), so any differences between the

      groups were not likely to be attributable to one group having more

      psychopathology than the other group. One of the main variables

      that distinguished those who died by suicide from living controls was

      62 ● WHY PEOPLE DIE BY SUICIDE

      a significant past history of deliberate self-harm.27 In a similar study,

      past attempts comprised a significant predictor of later death by sui-

      cide, even taking into account several other powerful predictors, such

      as presence of mood disorders.28 In these studies, multiple attempt

      status conferred risk to death by suicide, even beyond the effects of

      other powerful predictors, a finding quite consistent with the current

      conceptualization that people may “work up” to death by suicide

      through repeated episodes of deliberate self-harm (as well as through

      other means, noted later).

      Similarly, Boardman and colleagues compared those who died by

      suicide to controls who had died from other causes; cases and con-

      trols were matched for age and sex. Among the variables that distin-

      guished deaths by suicide from other deaths was a past history of de-

      liberate self-harm as well as a history of past criminal charges or

      contact with the police. Those who died by suicide had more sig-

      nificant histories of past self-harm and more police contact.29 The

      finding on criminality and legal contact is interesting; deliberate self-

      harm is the clearest means to habituate to self-injury, but not the


      only way. As will be expanded on later, other provocative experiences,

      including those associated with police contact (e.g., assault; injury

      from recklessness or substance abuse), may serve as well.

      A study following 529 mood-disordered patients over fourteen

      years found a similar pattern. Thirty-six participants died by suicide

      and 120 attempted suicide during the study. Among the variables

      that differentiated those who died by or attempted suicide from

      those who had no suicide attempt were history of previous attempts,

      impulsivity, and substance abuse.30 As with the previous finding on

      police contact, this result on substance abuse and impulsive behavior

      as precursors to suicidality is consistent with the view that an array

      of provocative experiences lays the groundwork for future self-injury.

      Another finding from this study was intriguing: Assertiveness was

      found to be a predictor of later suicidality among these mood-

      The Ability to Enact Lethal Self-Injury Is Acquired ● 63

      disordered patients. This finding on assertiveness as a predictor of

      suicidality squares with the current view that serious suicidality re-

      quires the accrual of a kind of courage or strength.

      My colleagues and I have recently conducted two studies that sup-

      port the conclusion that past suicidal behavior is related to future

      suicidal behavior in a fundamental and important way. We tried to

      take the same approach as some past investigators in that we assessed

      the relation of past suicidal behavior to later suicidality, again ac-

      counting for other key predictors. That is, we wanted to show that past and future suicidal behavior were related directly, as opposed to being associated simply because they are both related to a third thing,

      like a chronic mood disorder or personality disorder (this is known

      as “the third variable problem” in some research circles). The title of

      our paper included the phrase “the kitchen sink,” denoting our at-

      tempt to include as many “third variables” as we could think of. This

      paper included four different studies. A representative list of third

      variables would include: The demographic variables of age, marital

      status, and ethnicity; family history of suicide, depression, bipolar

      disorder, and alcohol abuse; personal history of legal trouble as an

      adult and as a juvenile; current and past diagnoses of depression and

     


    Prev Next
Online Read Free Novel Copyright 2016 - 2025