Online Read Free Novel
  • Home
  • Romance & Love
  • Fantasy
  • Science Fiction
  • Mystery & Detective
  • Thrillers & Crime
  • Actions & Adventure
  • History & Fiction
  • Horror
  • Western
  • Humor

    Why People Die By Suicide

    Prev Next


      The “resolved plans and preparations” category was made up of

      the following symptoms: a sense of courage to make an attempt; a

      sense of competence to make an attempt; availability of means to and

      opportunity for attempt; specificity of plan for attempt; prepara-

      tions for attempt; duration of suicidal ideation; and intensity of sui-

      cidal ideation. It is worth noticing that this category explicitly in-

      cludes indicators emphasizing courage and competence regarding

      The Ability to Enact Lethal Self-Injury Is Acquired ● 79

      suicide, which, according to the view proposed here, are reflective of

      the acquired ability to enact lethal self-injury.

      The “suicidal desire and ideation” category was comprised of the

      following symptoms: reasons for living; wish to die; frequency of

      ideation; wish not to live; passive attempt; desire for attempt; and

      talk of death/suicide. This factor does not include content related to

      courage, competence, and the like, but rather, emphasizes thwarted

      desire to live and reasons for death. In the next chapter, it will be ar-

      gued that thwarted desire to live can be understood in terms of feel-

      ing a burden on and disconnected from others. Thwarted desire to

      live is of course important in suicidality, but in an important sense, it

      is less clinically worrisome than the “resolved plans” category.

      Although the presence of symptoms corresponding to either cate-

      gory is of clinical concern, the symptoms of “resolved plans and

      preparations” are, relatively speaking, of more concern than the symp-

      toms of “suicidal desire and ideation.” And crucial to the predic-

      tion that serious suicidal symptoms should become more and more

      prominent with repeated suicidal experience, “resolved plans and

      preparations” was significantly more related than “suicidal desire and

      ideation” to status as a multiple suicide attempter.68

      A subsequent study reached similar conclusions regarding attempt

      status as well as eventual death by completed suicide. Specifically, my

      colleagues and I studied several hundred current suicide ideators,

      who were evaluated regarding their “worst-point” suicidal crisis, and

      then who were followed for many years.69 The “worst-point” was de-

      fined as a past suicidal crisis that was the most severe in the respon-

      dent’s life. In this study, as in the earlier reports, the distinction be-

      tween “resolved plans and preparations” (which includes courage

      and competence regarding suicide) and “desire for death” (the less

      serious dimension) was emphasized, in that respondents rated their

      “resolved plans” and their “desire for death” with regard to their

      worst-point crisis.

      80 ● WHY PEOPLE DIE BY SUICIDE

      According to the model proposed in this book, people who have

      experienced severe episodes of suicidality in the past (particularly

      if the episode involved loss of fear and other “resolved plans and

      preparations” phenomena) may be most at risk for severe suicidality

      in the future, and possibly even death by suicide. Our results con-

      formed to this view: The “worst-point resolved plans and prepara-

      tions” symptoms were the strongest predictor of suicide attempts

      during the follow-up period, and the only significant predictor of eventual death by suicide in the sample; the “suicidal desire and

      ideation” symptoms were not associated with later death by suicide.

      Consistent with these findings, a separate eighteen-year follow-up

      study found that planfulness regarding episodes of deliberate self-harm represented a significant risk factor for later completed sui-

      cide.70 Planfulness requires competence, which in my model is a key

      aspect of the acquired capability for lethal self-injury.

      That fearlessness and accrued courage and competence regarding

      suicide—key indicators of the “resolved plans and preparations” fac-

      tor—are implicated in severe suicidality is a central assertion of the

      present theory. In this connection, it is of interest to recall the four-

      teen-year prospective study of several hundred mood-disordered pa-

      tients mentioned earlier.71 Assertiveness was found to be a predictor

      of severe suicidality during the fourteen-year follow-up period. My

      model is perhaps the only theory of suicide that is compatible with a

      relation between assertiveness and suicidality.

      My colleagues and I analyzed notes written by those who died by

      suicide as well as those who attempted and survived72 using a soft-

      ware program called Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count (LIWC).73

      The program divides text into its components—for example, ten-

      dency to use action verbs, words denoting negative emotion, and so

      on. Among the clearest variables that differentiated notes by those

      who died by suicide from notes by those who attempted suicide and

      survived were indices related to assertiveness, specifically anger com-

      The Ability to Enact Lethal Self-Injury Is Acquired ● 81

      bined with confidence. The combination of anger and confidence

      bears some similarity to the combination of courage and resolution

      of ambivalence regarding suicide.

      Another aspect of the “resolved plans and preparations” category

      should be noted. In addition to indicators related to courage and

      competence regarding suicide, the factor also involved intense, vivid,

      and long-lasting ideation about one’s death by suicide. People who

      experience this say that they can see their death by suicide very

      clearly in their mind’s eye—it is as if they are watching a clear and

      vivid video of their own death by suicide. In this context, it is an

      interesting possibility that courage and competence regarding sui-

      cide may develop mentally as well as behaviorally. That is, vivid and

      long-lasting preoccupation regarding one’s suicide may represent a

      form of mental practice. To the extent that one rehearses for suicide,

      whether actually or mentally, suicide potential is increased. The con-

      cept of mental rehearsal may be helpful in understanding those who

      die by suicide on their first attempt—studies have found rates of

      first-attempt completed suicide as high as 50 percent.74 Mental prac-

      tice may facilitate suicide completion among those attempting it for

      the first time.

      Shneidman’s case example of Beatrice illustrates this aspect of

      mental practice. She says, “For the next two years . . . every night, be-

      fore fading off to sleep, I imagined committing suicide. I became ob-

      sessed with death. I rehearsed my own funeral over and over, adding

      careful details each time.”75 Beatrice later planned her suicide for

      three months, and tried to die by self-cutting; she survived.

      In 1992 and 1993, musician Kurt Cobain obsessively watched a

      videotape of the suicide of R. Budd Dwyer, a Pennsylvania state of-

      ficial who died at a live press conference (that Dwyer himself had

      called) by putting a gun in his mouth and firing.76 This may have

      represented a form of mental practice for Cobain’s 1994 death by a

      similar method.

      82 ● WHY PEOPLE DIE BY SUICIDE


      David Reimer, mentioned earlier and described in the book As

      Nature Made Him: The Boy Who Was Raised as a Girl, said that in

      eighth grade, when he was living as a girl, he “kept visualizing a rope

      thrown over a beam.”77 He would have continued experience with

      suicidality as well as numerous other provocative experiences, and at

      age thirty-eight, died by suicide.

      A study of over 3,000 patients at risk for suicide, thirty-eight of

      whom subsequently died by suicide, provides some indirect evidence

      regarding mental practice.78 Of the factors that predicted death by

      suicide, an important one was “contemplation of hanging or jump-

      ing.” Through mental rehearsal of violent death by suicide, these

      patients may have acquired more of the ability to enact lethal self-

      injury.

      Also relevant here is the concept of aborted suicide attempts, de-

      fined as an event in which an individual comes close to attempting

      suicide but does not do so and thus sustains no injury. Barber and

      colleagues interviewed 135 psychiatric inpatients, and over half re-

      ported at least one aborted suicide attempt.79 Intent-to-die ratings

      for aborted suicide attempts were similar to those for actual suicide

      attempts, indicating that aborted attempts can have severe quali-

      ties—qualities that could potentially produce habituation and prac-

      tice effects. Moreover, patients who reported aborted attempts were

      nearly twice as likely to have made an actual suicide attempt as pa-

      tients with no aborted attempts. Practice regarding suicide may oc-

      cur in the absence of actual suicidal behavior, either through mental

      rehearsal or through aborted suicide attempts.

      One additional reason to worry about the accrual of courage

      about suicide relates to the concept of cognitive sensitization. Cog-

      nitive sensitization occurs when one undergoes a provocative experi-

      ence, and subsequently, images and thoughts about that experience

      become more accessible and easily triggered. As applied to suicid-

      ality, as suicidal experience accumulates, suicide-related cognitions

      The Ability to Enact Lethal Self-Injury Is Acquired ● 83

      and behaviors may become more accessible and active.80 The more

      accessible and active these thoughts and behaviors become, the more

      easily they are triggered (e.g., even in the absence of negative events),

      and the more severe are the subsequent suicidal episodes. My col-

      leagues and I have documented that, in fact, as episodes of suicidality

      increase, their relation to external triggers decreases, and their sever-

      ity increases.81

      Vicarious Habituation: The Example of Guns in the Home

      As the example of aborted suicide attempts shows, there are multiple

      ways that people may habituate to dangerous stimuli. One way is

      through habituating to danger by observing someone else do so, or

      by repeatedly being exposed to cues associated with danger. The ex-

      ample of Kurt Cobain’s obsessively watching a videotaped suicide

      was mentioned earlier, as was his gradually increasing interest in

      guns. Guns in the home are an issue in this regard.

      First, let me disclose that I am not a huge fan of guns—I don’t

      own one myself—but neither do I have strong feelings against gun

      ownership. Regardless of one’s viewpoint on this topic, there ap-

      pears to be an undeniable association between the presence of guns

      in a home and suicides occurring in that home. For example, a study

      across twenty-one countries documented this association very

      clearly.82 In twelve countries, another study found that the percent-

      age of households with guns was strongly associated with the over-

      all mortality rate from guns in children aged 0–15 years, including

      death by suicide.83 An association does not prove a causal connection

      between presence of guns and suicide, but the association is consis-

      tent with the possibility that having guns around acquaints people—

      renders them fearless—about a potentially lethal stimulus.

      Brent and colleagues did an interesting study on whether families

      with a depressed adolescent follow recommendations to remove

      guns from the house. Of families advised to remove firearms from

      84 ● WHY PEOPLE DIE BY SUICIDE

      their homes, 26.9 percent did so. Interestingly, the decision to keep

      a gun, even when advised not to, was associated with the father’s

      psychopathology as well as marital dissatisfaction.84

      Repetition May Reinforce Suicidal Behavior

      The singer Pink, who has numerous body piercings and tattoos, said

      in the December 2003 issue of Jane magazine, “I took out my tongue ring when I was 21 and regretted it a week later. I like putting holes in

      my body. It’s addictive, it’s pain to know you’re alive.”85 The evidence

      summarized so far suggests that habituation and practice effects may

      be implicated in the escalating trajectory toward serious suicidality.

      In addition to habituation and practice, the theory put forth here

      suggests that repeated suicidality may engage opponent processes,

      such that not only do people habituate to self-injury, they also come

      to experience it as increasingly rewarding, similar to the way Pink re-

      ported that “putting holes” in her body is addictive.

      Many people appear to share Pink’s perspective. There is clear and

      consistent evidence that a primary motive for self-injury is relief,

      and that people find self-injury rewarding, at least in the immediate

      period following the incident. This may seem hard to imagine, but

      recall the example of skydiving. In a way, flinging yourself out of

      an airplane makes no more sense than cutting the side of your arm—

      indeed skydiving deaths occur every year. Why do people do this,

      then? The first time they skydive, they feel some of the thrill and

      exhilaration of it, and a large dose of the fear of it. But as they

      keep doing it, encouraged by the thrill and exhilaration, the pri-

      mary process of fear fades, and the opponent process of exhilaration

      strengthens.

      So it goes with self-injury. As people continue to do it, the primary

      process of pain fades, and the opponent process amplifies. What is

      the opponent process? As noted earlier, according to patients who

      self-harm, it is relief because it distracts from even deeper emotional

      The Ability to Enact Lethal Self-Injury Is Acquired ● 85

      pain, or because it makes them feel alive, or because it brings their

      inner world back into harmony with the world at large.

      Several studies support this idea.86 These findings appear to impli-

      cate what is called negative reinforcement (i.e., the self-injury is rein-

      forcing because it stops or reduces noxious experiences). However,

      positive reinforcement (i.e., the self-injury is reinforcing because it

      induces positive feelings) may be operative as well—for example, in a

      study of female psychiatric inpatients with borderline personality

      disorder (a main feature of which is repeated self-harm), patients

      rated various dimensions of their self-injury experiences. Results re-

      vealed significant mood elevation
    as a consequence of self-injury

      among these patients.87 Many patients report that although negative

      reinforcement (i.e., relief) is a primary motive for self-injury,88 other

      motives exist, including positive reinforcers such as fascination with

      the injury and reaffirming the ability to feel89—or, in Pink’s words,

      “pain to know you’re alive.”

      To my knowledge, however, only one study has directly evaluated

      whether the rewarding properties of self-injury increase with repeti-

      tion. Participants who frequently engaged in self-injury were com-

      pared to those who infrequently did so, with regard to responses

      to a self-mutilation imagery task. In response to the imagery task,

      those in the frequent self-injury group reported more relief and

      more reductions in anxiety and sadness as compared to the infre-

      quent group.90 As people continue to engage in self-injury (or un-

      dergo other provocative experiences), they change. Self-injury loses

      its painful and fear-inducing properties and may even begin to gain

      rewarding properties. As this occurs, the main barrier to suicide

      erodes.

      The Psychological Merging of Death and Life

      To this point, the argument has been made that those prone to seri-

      ous suicidal behavior have reached that status through a process of

      86 ● WHY PEOPLE DIE BY SUICIDE

      exposure to self-injury and other provocative experiences. As this

      process unfolds, fear of death and pain on self-injury decreases. As

      reviewed above, certain scientific facts seem to support this view.

      Little has been said so far about how potentially suicidal people

      view death (except that they come not to fear it). When someone is

      far along the trajectory toward suicide, when they have acquired the

      ability to enact lethal self-injury, what is their view of death? Though

      there are very few scientific data on this point, anecdotal and case

      summary data suggest that people who are near death by suicide

      view death in a very peculiar way—namely, that death is somehow

      life-giving.

      For most people, the notion that death is life-giving or nurturing

      is not only irrational but very disturbing. Suicidal people appear to

      see it differently, however. For example, in Shneidman’s case study of

      Ariel, she stated, “We were in this old cemetery, and what was inter-

      esting and unique about this cemetery is that it is very old and the

     


    Prev Next
Online Read Free Novel Copyright 2016 - 2025